Skip to main content

SSR (Sustained Silent Reading)


Stephen Krashen of comprehensible input renown is a strong supporter of silent reading in class. He and fellow advocates of SSR point to a reasonable range of empirical studies which suggest that children who do a programme of SSR go on to do better in reading tests. You might say that that is no great surprise - generally you get better at what you practise.

English departments often incorporate a library lesson or silent reading lesson in their schemes of work. Why don't MFL departments?

Well, firstly there are severe time constraints on teachers to get through the syllabus in time frames that are already too narrow. It is not unusual for classes to have only two or three contacts per week (sometimes even less), so teachers feel reluctant to devote time to independent reading.

Secondly, there is a dearth (total absence?) of suitable reading material which would provide appropriate comprehensible input. There used to be. I and many colleagues made good use of the Bibliobus series in French. These were illustrated graded readers which students could work through on their own. I recall a perky and charming Year 7 girl asking me, as they all read silently, "Don't you get bored,sir, when we're reading?"

Thirdly, I doubt whether many teachers really buy into the claim that silent reading leads to significant gains in second language acquisition. they feel that they are only doing their job properly when they are providing input themselves and directing the learning. I bet you no teacher would plan a silent reading lesson for an Ofsted inspection either, even though it would be a perfectly good lesson. (That tells you something about Ofsted.)

If schools allocated enough regular time to languages, at least four contacts a week, and if publsihers produced the right material, either in book form or online, then I would still advocate sessions of sustained silent reading. In the end, whilst I don't necessarily go along with Prof Krashen is every respect, I still think acquisition comes down primarily to quality input.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

What is the natural order hypothesis?

The natural order hypothesis states that all learners acquire the grammatical structures of a language in roughly the same order. This applies to both first and second language acquisition. This order is not dependent on the ease with which a particular language feature can be taught; in English, some features, such as third-person "-s" ("he runs") are easy to teach in a classroom setting, but are not typically fully acquired until the later stages of language acquisition. The hypothesis was based on morpheme studies by Heidi Dulay and Marina Burt, which found that certain morphemes were predictably learned before others during the course of second language acquisition. The hypothesis was picked up by Stephen Krashen who incorporated it in his very well known input model of second language learning. Furthermore, according to the natural order hypothesis, the order of acquisition remains the same regardless of the teacher's explicit instruction; in other words,

What is skill acquisition theory?

For this post, I am drawing on a section from the excellent book by Rod Ellis and Natsuko Shintani called Exploring Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (Routledge, 2014). Skill acquisition is one of several competing theories of how we learn new languages. It’s a theory based on the idea that skilled behaviour in any area can become routinised and even automatic under certain conditions through repeated pairing of stimuli and responses. When put like that, it looks a bit like the behaviourist view of stimulus-response learning which went out of fashion from the late 1950s. Skill acquisition draws on John Anderson’s ACT theory, which he called a cognitivist stimulus-response theory. ACT stands for Adaptive Control of Thought.  ACT theory distinguishes declarative knowledge (knowledge of facts and concepts, such as the fact that adjectives agree) from procedural knowledge (knowing how to do things in certain situations, such as understand and speak a language).

12 principles of second language teaching

This is a short, adapted extract from our book The Language Teacher Toolkit . "We could not possibly recommend a single overall method for second language teaching, but the growing body of research we now have points to certain provisional broad principles which might guide teachers. Canadian professors Patsy Lightbown and Nina Spada (2013), after reviewing a number of studies over the years to see whether it is better to just use meaning-based approaches or to include elements of explicit grammar teaching and practice, conclude: Classroom data from a number of studies offer support for the view that form-focused instruction and corrective feedback provided within the context of communicative and content-based programmes are more effective in promoting second language learning than programmes that are limited to a virtually exclusive emphasis on comprehension. As teachers Gianfranco and I would go along with that general view and would like to suggest our own set of g